(chapter & of my book Believing Bullshit available UK here (US amazon.com here) [below is the original, uncorrected text]). New Scientist interview with me about the book here.
An anecdote involves the recounting of a short story or episode,
supposedly true, and often testimonial in nature. There’s nothing wrong with
anecdotes per se—they can usefully be
used to spice up a dinner party conversation, provoke a discussion or
illustrate a point. I’ve told a few in my time. However, alarm bells should
start ringing whenever anecdotes are supposed to provide significant evidence in support of a claim,
particularly a supernatural claim. Here are a few examples:
I know I’m psychic. For
example, last week I was thinking about Aunt Sue, whom
I hadn’t talked to for
ages, when the phone rang. And it was her.
Prayer clearly works. I
prayed for Mark, John, Karen, and Rita and they all got better.
I have no doubt that
ghosts are real. My mother saw one just last week. And she’s
a trustworthy
woman not prone to making things up.
Anecdotal evidence is also a staple
of snake-oil salesmen everywhere, who can usually produce a handful of
supporting testimonials to the efficacy of their remedies:
John ate three of my patented
magic beans, and his cancer disappeared. Here’s his sworn testimony!
People are attracted to anecdotes.
We especially love hearing tales of the extraordinary and supernatural. Many of
us are easily swayed by anecdotal evidence for the existence of psychic powers,
ghosts, or the efficacy of prayer or of some alternative medicine. Yet, as
evidence, anecdotes are almost entirely worthless. Why? For a range of reasons.
Here are a few examples.
Amazing Coincidence
First of all, note that amazing coincidences are inevitable. There are
billions of people living on this planet, each experiencing thousands of events
each day. Inevitably, some of them are going to experience some really
remarkable coincidences.
Such coincidences will be thrown up
by chance. The odds of flipping a coin and getting a run of ten heads by chance
is very low if you only flip the coin ten times. But if billions of people do
the same thing, it becomes very likely indeed that a run of ten heads will
occur.
Such coincidences can easily
generate the appearance of supernatural activity. For example, such
coincidences can suggest that prayer can cure people of terminal diseases.
Among people diagnosed with terminal cancer, a small percentage will
spontaneously get better. Such rare occurrences are just a natural fact about
cancer. Huge numbers of people are diagnosed with terminal cancer each year.
And a significant proportion of them are prayed for. It’s likely, then, that a
few of those diagnosed with terminal cancer and prayed for will recover. Is the
existence of such people evidence that prayer works? Clearly not. These are
people who would have gotten better anyway, prayed for or not. A handful of
reports of such amazing recoveries is not good evidence of the efficacy of petitionary
prayer.
What would be more impressive is if,
say, after being prayed for, someone grew an amputated leg back. That’s
something that really would run contrary to everything we know about how our
bodies function. If, in response to prayer, God, really did heal people by
supernatural means, and if his powers are unlimited, then he could just as
easily grow someone a new leg as cure them of terminal cancer. However,
well-documented cases of people growing legs back after being prayed for do
not, so far as I am aware, exist. Interestingly, reports of “miraculous”
medical recoveries tend largely to be restricted to the kinds of case
in which such spontaneous remission is known to occur.
What about the phone-ringing
episode? Just the other day, I was booked to play at a wedding in some fairly
remote countryside about fifty miles from where I live. When I arrived, my
brother walked out of the building to meet me. He was as amazed to see me as I
was to see him. The venue was miles away from where either of us lived. But, by
sheer chance, we ended up at the same place at the same time. A month or two
ago, my wife took a train journey to a station in the North of England. When
she stepped onto the platform, her father was standing there. Again, both were
amazed. Again, this was a coincidence. The fact is, coincidences happen. Every
now and again, people will run into each other in unexpected locations. Every
now and then, the phone will ring and at the end of the line will be someone
you were just thinking about.
Coincidence also accounts for at
least some sightings of monsters. Consider the many thousands of people who
look out over Loch Ness each year. All sorts of shapes are created in the water
by floating logs, otters, wind patterns, the wakes of boats, and so on. Just by
chance, a few will look a little monster-like. So, if a monster is what people
are looking for, we should expect a few such reports of a monster, whether
there’s a monster in the loch or not.
Those finding hidden codes in ancient
texts also tend to rely heavily on coincidence. In the book The Bible Code, journalist Michael
Drosnin claims to have discovered within the Bible a code revealing events that
happened thousands of years after the text was written, events such as the
assassination of John F. Kennedy at Dallas. Drosnin also claims no mere human
could have encoded these hidden messages, and that he has therefore discovered
mathematical proof that “we are not alone.”1
Has Drosnin really discovered such
deliberately hidden predictive messages within the pages of The Bible? Critics note that Drosnin’s
method of revealing his messages looks suspiciously as if it would throw them
up by chance. Drosnin denies this. He claimed in Newsweek, “When my critics find a message about the assassination
of a prime minister encrypted in Moby
Dick, I’ll believe them.”2
One critic then proceeded to do just
that. Mathematician Brendan MacKay subsequently used Drosnin’s method to find
encrypted in Moby Dick “predictions”
of the assassinations of Leon Trotsky, Indira Ghandi, Rev. Martin Luther King
Jr., Rene Moawad, and Robert F. Kennedy. In fact it turns out that by using
Drosnin’s method you can find such “messages” hidden in any large text. They’re
thrown up by chance among the vast number of letter sequences that Drosnin’s
method generates.
The important thing to remember
about coincidences is this—what would be really
odd is if they didn’t happen. If no
one ever unexpectedly ran into a friend or relative, or if we never received
phone calls from people we just happened to be thinking about—well that really
would be pretty peculiar. The fact that amazing coincidences happen is, or
should be, entirely unsurprising, and requires no supernatural explanation.
The Post Hoc Fallacy
People often assume that because one thing happens after another, that
one is the cause of the other. But,
actually, there needn’t be any causal link. To assume that, because B followed
A, A caused B is to commit the
fallacy known as post hoc ergo propter
hoc (which means, “After this, therefore because of this”). Suppose my
kettle boils immediately after a comet crashes into Mars. Did the comet cause
my kettle to boil? No. That’s just a coincidence.
Similarly, the fact that someone
diagnosed with terminal cancer recovers after prayer does not establish that
prayer caused the recovery. To suppose otherwise is also to commit the post hoc
fallacy.
Recovery after diagnosis of terminal
cancer might be an amazing one-off coincidence. But what if we spot a pattern? What if, whenever A happens, B always, or very often, follows. Would that establish that A causes B?
Suppose, for example, that some New
Age medical treatment advertised like so: 90 percent of those suffering from
unexplained lower back pain who took magic beans as treatment reported a
significant improvement after just a few weeks!
Wow, that sounds impressive—90 percent! Surely we have here evidence
that magic beans really do effectively treat lower back pain?
No, we don’t. Ninety percent of
cases of unexplained lower back pain will have improved significantly after six
weeks, even with no treatment at all.
So the fact that 90 percent of those with unexplained lower back pain improve
significantly after receiving magic beans, crystal healing, homeopathy, or a
rubdown with pink blancmange is no evidence at all that any of these treatments
have any sort of beneficial effect.
What we tend to overlook is the
extent to which the supposed “effect” happens anyway, whether or not the alleged “cause” is present. One hundred
percent of those people who drink water eventually die. That doesn’t establish
that drinking water is the cause of their death.
Counting the Hits and Ignoring
the Misses
Francis Bacon, a pivotal figure in the development of the modern
scientific method, once said, “The general root of superstition is that men
observe when things hit, and not when they miss; and commit to memory the one,
and forget and pass over the other.” Anecdotes can appear to provide compelling
evidence of psychic abilities and supernatural events, particularly when many
are collected together in a book or article. Page upon page of anecdotes about
the amazing insights of psychics can leave people thinking, “Well, there’s got
to be something to it, surely!” But
how is this evidence accumulated? Typically, as Bacon notes, people look for cases that seem to support the
theory they believe in, and ignore those that don’t. This is called confirmation bias.
For example, someone who believes
they are psychic will usually focus on the few “hits,” e.g., those times when
they received a phone call from someone they just thought about. They forget
about the many “misses”: all those times when they thought about someone but
the person didn’t immediately ring. By collecting together several such “hits”
and ignoring the innumerable “misses,” it’s not difficult to convince yourself
that you have psychic powers, even if you don’t.
Similarly, someone who believes in
the efficacy of prayer will typically ignore all those cases in which people
diagnosed with terminal cancer were prayed for and didn’t recover—the
overwhelming majority—and will focus exclusively on the handful of cases where
there was a full recovery. By ignoring the “misses”—all those occasions on
which sick people were prayed for but they experienced no recovery, and collect
together only on the “hits,” the small proportion of occasions the person
recovered, we can, again, easily convince ourselves that that we have amassed
powerful evidence of the miraculous efficacy of prayer.
We can now see more clearly one of
the main reasons why anecdotal evidence is such poor evidence. When we are
simply presented with a large collection of anecdotes, we have no idea how idiosyncratic the cases are. If I
casually take a sawn-off shotgun and pepper the side of a barn on which a small
target is hanging, and a couple of shotgun pellets happen to fall inside the
target, that’s not evidence of my great marksmanship. Someone that initially
only looks at the two holes in the target might be impressed, but once they
take a step back and see all the misses, it becomes obvious that there’s no
evidence of marksmanship after all. The “hits” were highly atypical.
Not only do we tend to count the
“hits” and forget about the “misses,” we also tend, when recounting anecdotes,
to focus on those features that make the story sound dramatic and downplay
details that make it less so. There’s often also an incentive to “sex up” anecdotes—sometimes
even a financial incentive. Tabloid newspapers and TV production companies know
that, as a rule, their audiences tend to be more interested in dramatic and
extraordinary tales than in articles or programs that shed doubts on such
stories. As a result, even while pretending to be “balanced,” TV programs on
the paranormal are often little more than puffs for self-styled psychics.
Doubts, if voiced at all, tend to be in the background. As a result of all this
anecdote generating and peddling by the media, many people have become convinced
there is abundant evidence that ghosts exist, that some people really are
blessed with psychic powers, that some people have been abducted by aliens, and
so on.
The Power of Suggestion
and Our Tendency to “See” What Is Not There
Human beings are remarkably prone to “see” things that are not, in
truth, there. Take, for example, the
power of suggestion, nicely illustrated by Kenneth Arnold’s famous sighting
of the very first flying saucer back in 1963. Arnold was flying his light plane
near Mount Rainier in Washington when he saw a series of mysterious objects. On
landing, he reported these unidentified flying objects. The news media picked
up the story of Arnold’s flying saucers, and, soon after, very many other
people were reporting the saucer-shaped objects in the sky. They have been
reporting them ever since. The saucer-shaped spacecraft has become a staple of
science fiction. But here’s the thing—Arnold
did not report seeing flying saucers. What Arnold said he saw were
boomerang-shaped craft that bobbed up and down, somewhat like a saucer would if
skimmed across a lake. The reporter misheard, the story of “flying saucers”
entered the public sphere, and other people started reporting saucers too. Why?
Assuming most of them were sincere, and assuming it’s unlikely our alien
visitors just happened to switch from using boomerang-shaped craft to
saucer-shaped craft in 1963, it seems the saucer reports that followed were,
and are, largely a product of the power of suggestion. People see something in
the sky, and, because they expect it to be saucer shaped, that’s how it looks
to them. Expectation strongly shapes perception.
Rigorous investigation of reports of
unidentified flying objects has thrown up numerous examples of how our eyes can
deceive us. In the autumn of 1967 there was a rash of reports of a UFO
appearing nightly over the construction site of a nuclear plant. Sanitation
workers reported it, then a guard. The police showed up. An officer confirmed,
“It was about half the size of the moon, and it just hung there over the plant.
Must have been there nearly two hours.” The strange object disappeared at
sunrise. The next night the same thing happened. A county deputy sheriff
described a “large lighted object.” An auxiliary police officer described “five
objects—they appeared to be burning. An aircraft passed by while I was
watching. They seemed to be 20 times the size of the plane.” A Wake County
magistrate saw “a rectangular object, looked like it was on fire. . . . We
figured it about the size of a football field. It was huge and very bright.”
There was also a report from air traffic control of an unidentified blip on the
radar scope.
When newspaper reporters arrived to
investigate the mysterious object, it appeared again at 5 am. The reporters attempted to chase it
in a car. They discovered that no matter how fast they drove they couldn’t get
any closer. Finally, they stopped to take pictures of the mysterious object.
The photographer looked through his long telephoto lens and said, “Yep . . .
that’s the planet Venus alright.”3
Once the planet had been mistaken
for a large hovering object by one person, well, that’s how everyone else saw
it too, until, finally, someone finally looked at it through a magnifying lens
and realized the truth. You might be surprised to discover that Venus is one of
the biggest sources of UFO reports. Anyone who thinks that a group of honest,
experienced, trained eye-witnesses—police officers, no less—can’t be seriously
and repeatedly misled by the power of suggestion should think again. Also
notice how coincidence threw into the mix of this story an apparent
“independent” confirmation—that spurious radar blip.
It’s not just visual perception that’s
affected by the power of suggestion. A auditory example, widely available on
the internet, is provided by the song “Stairway to Heaven” by Led Zeppelin, one
passage of which, when played backwards (easy to do on an old-style record
player), is supposed to say:
Oh here’s to my sweet Satan.
The one whose little path would make
me sad, whose power is Satan.
He will give those with him 666.
There was a little tool shed where
he made us suffer, sad Satan.
Actually, listen to the song
backwards without having seen the suggested lyrics (obviously, I’ve ruined this
for you now) and people can’t make out much at all, except maybe one or two
words, such as “Satan.” Play the reversed passage to people with these words in
front of them, on the other hand, and they find it almost impossible not to hear the words.
How did the myth of the hidden
message in “Stairway to Heaven” arise? Someone playing rock records
backwards—either messing about, or actually looking for hidden messages—came
across what sounds like the dramatic and noteworthy word “Satan” (thrown up by
chance) in “Stairway to Heaven”, and then constructed lyrics suggested to them
by the surrounding noises. Having produced the satanic lyrics, the more they
listened, the more obvious it seemed to them the words were really there. The
truth, of course, is that the satanic lyrics people “hear” are a product of the
minds of listeners, not the mind of Led Zep’s lyricist Robert Plant.
Even setting aside the power of
suggestion, various other factors can shape perception, including our obvious perceptual
sensitivity to faces. Look up at passing cumulus clouds, or stare into the
embers of a fire, and all sorts of things start to appear. By far the most
common are faces. We are naturally attuned to them, and can easily “find” a
face in most randomly generated patterns.
In 1976, the space probe Viking
Orbiter 1 was busy photographing the Cydonia region of Mars. On July 25 it took
a picture of what appeared to many to be an enormous alien face carved onto the
planet’s surface. The Mars Face, as it become known, caused much speculation.
One author, Richard Haugland, suggested in his book The Monuments of Mars: City On the Edge of Forever, that the reptilian-looking
face was a vast monument created by some ancient Martian civilization, the
Martian equivalent of the Great Pyramid of Giza. However, other photographs of
the same region reveal that the Mars Face is just a hill that doesn’t look very
face-like at all unless lit at a certain angle, when it happens by chance to
take on a face-like appearance.
In fact, the Mars Face is a product
of two factors: (1) Chance eventually threw up a rather face-like set of
shadows among the hundreds of photographs of a planet’s surface. This face-like
image was then further enhanced by (2) our tendency to “see” faces in such
patterns anyway. These same two factors account for the many reports of
mysterious faces appearing in things. If you have five minutes to spare, a
quick trawl through the internet will reveal Mother Theresa’s face in a bun,
Jesus’ face on the back of a bedroom door, and a demon’s face appearing in a
cloud of smoke emerging from the Twin Towers.
The placebo effect provides another
example of the power of suggestion. During the Second World War, anesthetist
Henry Beecher, faced with a lack of morphine at a military field hospital,
tried a rather desperate ploy. He injected a wounded soldier with inert saline
solution, but told the soldier it was a powerful painkiller. Amazingly, the
soldier relaxed and stopped exhibiting signs of significant pain or distress.
When Beecher repeated the ploy on other soldiers, he got the same effect. We
are remarkably prone to the power of suggestion when it comes to medical
treatment. Tell people something will make them better—that it will relieve
their pain, give their joints better mobility, reduce their acne, or
whatever—and they’ll believe, and report in all sincerity, that it does. The
placebo effect, as it’s known, can create the illusion that a treatment is
medically effective when it is not. However, it can also contribute to the
effectiveness of even bona fide medicines.
Beecher subsequently went on to
publish a seminal paper, “The Powerful Placebo,”4 in which he argued for the importance of
conducting double-blind,
placebo-controlled clinical trials of treatments to establish their efficacy.
If we want to know whether, say, homeopathic remedies have any effect other
than placebo, we need two large groups into which individuals have been
randomly assigned, one group receiving the homeopathic drug, the other the
medically inert placebo. The trial should be double blind: the subjects should
not know who is receiving the genuine treatment and who the inert alternative.
The experimenters should also be blind to this information, in order to counter
the “experimenter effect” (it is well established that experimenters can
inadvertently influence the outcome of such trials if they know who is and isn’t
receiving the genuine treatment). Unfortunately for homeopathy, such
well-conducted trials have failed to provide any convincing evidence of the efficacy
of homeopathic treatments for any particular ailment.
It is not just perception that can
be led astray by the power of suggestion. The psychologist Jean Piaget once
claimed his earliest memory was of nearly being kidnapped at the age of two
while being walked in his pram by his nurse:
I can still see, most clearly, the
following scene, in which I believed until I was about fifteen. I was sitting
in my pram, which my nurse was pushing in the Champs Elysées, when a man tried
to kidnap me. I was held in by the strap fastened round me while my nurse
bravely tried to stand between me and the thief. She received various
scratches, and I can still see vaguely those on her face. Then a crowd
gathered, a policeman with a short cloak and a white baton came up, and the man
took to his heels. I can still see the whole scene, and can even place it near
the tube station.
Later, when Piaget was about
fifteen, his family received a letter in which the nurse admitted the story was
false:
She had made up the whole story,
faking the scratches. I, therefore, must have heard, as a child, the account of
this story, which my parents believed, and projected into the past in the form
of a visual memory.5
Studies reveal that in somewhere
between18 to 37 percent of subjects researchers can successfully “implant”
false memories of events such as animal attacks, riding in a hot air balloon
with one’s family, and witnessing a demonic possession.6
Other Mechanisms:
Chinese Whispers, Fraud and Fakery, Etc.
Another factor that further undermines the credibility of much anecdotal
evidence is what I call the Chinese
whispers effect. When amazing tales are transmitted from one person to
another, the retellings often involve some subtle or not so subtle editing.
Those details that are dramatic tend to be remembered and exaggerated. Those
that undermine the credibility of the anecdote tend to be airbrushed out. Even
if each reteller reshapes the original story only slightly, it takes only a
handful of retellings for the story to change significantly. So we can place
even less credence in stories that reach us fourth, fifth, or sixth hand.
We should also remember that, when
it comes to anecdotes about faith healing, spoon bending, mind reading,
communication with the dead, and so on, many people have been revealed as
frauds. In 1983, Christian healer Peter Popoff, who regularly “cured” people of
serious illnesses during his revival meetings, was exposed as a cheat by
magician James Randi. Popoff would wheel subjects on to the stage in
wheelchairs, subjects who were then miraculously able to walk. It turned out these
were people who could already walk that Popoff had simply brought on in
wheelchairs. Popoff was also caught receiving information on audience members
given to him by his wife via a radio earpiece.
The list of fakes and frauds is
long, and includes the three Fox sisters, who helped generate huge
mid-nineteenth century interest in communication with the dead. The sisters
conduct séances in New York in which the dead would communicate by making
rapping noises. The Foxes performed in public theaters, and their work
attracted many notable people. Two of the sisters were later to admit
“perpetrating the fraud of Spiritualism upon a too-confiding public.”7 Though they later retracted their
confessions, Margaret had nevertheless demonstrated how she could produce the
mysterious raps by cracking her toe joints at will.
Not all of the claims made about the
Fox sisters’ séances were, however, a result of fraud. In some cases, the
public were to add dramatic details of their own. Margaret was to say:
A great many people when they hear
the rapping imagine at once that the spirits are touching them. It is a very
common delusion. Some very wealthy people came to see me some years ago when I
lived in Forty-second Street and I did some rappings for them. I made the spirit
rap on the chair and one of the ladies cried out: “I feel the spirit tapping me
on the shoulder.” Of course that was pure imagination.”8
Another form of fakery by psychics
and mediums is the use of hot and/or cold reading. Hot reading involves research
in advance. A psychic may prepare for a reading by researching their client on
the internet. Some psychics will place stooges in the foyers of theaters in
which they are performing to overhear the conversations of audience members, make
notes, and pass information back to the psychic. Sometimes the person for whom
a public reading is done will be known to the psychic, or someone close to the
psychic, who may then pass on information. Sometimes stooges will join the
audience, pretending to be ordinary members of the public.
Cold reading is more of an art form,
and involves creating the illusion that the psychic knows things about their
subject. Psychic readings typically begin like so:
PSYCHIC: I am getting someone whose
name begins with “G.” George . . .
[pause] . . . Or Gerald.
CUSTOMER: Gerald! My uncle’s name
was Gerald.
PSYCHIC: Yes, Gerald is here with me
now. He is saying Hello!
CUSTOMER: That’s amazing!
PSYCHIC: He being quite shy, quite
coy.
CUSTOMER: [No reaction]
PSYCHIC: Which is odd, because he
was such an outgoing chap, wasn’t he?
CUSTOMER: Yes, that’s right. He
loved the social club.
PSYCHIC: Ah, yes, he was just saying
he missed his friends at there.
CUSTOMER. [Gets a little weepy] It’s
really him!
PSYCHIC: I’m sensing he had some
back trouble.
CUSTOMER: Yes he did! A slipped
disc.
PSYCHIC: That’s right. He says that
disc is all better now.
This customer may go away and tell
her friends that the psychic knew she had a dead uncle called Gerald who was
outgoing, missed his friends at the social club, and had a slipped disc. Her
friends may well be amazed and think that perhaps there’s something to this psychic business after all.
However, our psychic, in reality,
knew nothing. Let’s go through the reading again. The psychic tries a name. No
reaction. Then another, and gets a hit. But she does not say whether Gerald is
living or dead (it could be a message concerning a living person called
“Gerald”). It’s the customer who supplies the information that she has a dead
uncle of that name. The psychic then suggests Gerald is shy. No reaction, so
the psychic switches to saying Gerald was outgoing, and gets another hit. The
customer supplies the information that Gerald attended a social club. The
psychic then suggests Gerald had back trouble. “So-and-so had back trouble” is
what is known as a Barnum statement.
It sounds pretty specific, but is actually true of most people. Almost everyone
has back trouble at some point, so it’s not surprising the psychic gets another
hit. Other examples of Barnum statements are: “You had an accident when you
were a child involving water” and “You have been worrying about money
recently.” Psychics will typically make lots of Barnum statements. But notice
that even if Gerald’s back was always problem free, the psychic can switch
tactics and say, “No, sorry, I misheard—Gerald is saying you have had some back trouble.” Even if that fails to score a hit,
chances are the customer will quickly forget about it. As we have already
noted, it’s the hits we remember—the misses are soon forgotten.
By using a combination of hot and
cold reading, professional magicians can fake everything supposedly genuine
psychics can do—often fooling audiences into believing they are genuinely
psychic before revealing the truth. It is striking how closely the methods of
the supposedly genuine psychics mirror the methods of such honest cheats.
However, it would be a mistake
immediately to conclude that everyone who believes they are psychic—and who
presents him or herself as a psychic—is a fraud. Several years ago, a friend
and I played a simple mind-reading trick on another friend of ours. It’s a
simple trick you can try yourself. One person holds up a playing card and then
mentally “transmits” the color of the card to the another, who has to guess the
color. To the amazement of onlookers, the guesser keeps getting it right. It
looks like they are “mind reading,” but actually they are using a simple code:
when the person holding the card says “OK,” the card is black, and when they
say “Right,” it’s red. It’s a fairly obvious deception, and astonishing that
people fall for it. But many do, especially if you set the trick up so that it
seems to emerge as a bit spontaneous larking about. Throw in a few misses to
give the scenario credibility, and the trick works better still.
What was interesting on this
occasion was that, after impressing our victim with our psychic powers, we
decided to test her to see if she, too, could read the mind of the card holder.
She found, to her astonishment, that she could. She got more and more excited
about her amazing psychic ability, until we finally had to disappoint her by
revealing the truth—that she was merely subliminally picking up on the OK/Right
code.
Just like my friend, some psychics
lacking any genuine psychic ability may nevertheless sincerely believe they are
psychic. They may be picking up on all sorts of entirely natural signals and
clues without realizing they are doing so.
We may also unwittingly provide
psychics with information through our body language. Consider the strange case
of Clever Hans, a horse that could apparently perform mathematical
calculations—tapping out the answers with his hoof. In 1907, the psychologist
Oskar Pfungst conducted an investigation into the horse’s alleged mathematical
abilities and discovered that the horse was not doing math, but picking up on
the very subtle reactions of his human trainer. The trainer was, without
realizing it, cueing the horse when to stop tapping. When the trainer did not
know the answer, it turned out that neither did the horse. The Clever Hans
effect, as it become known, illustrates how we can “leak” information without
realizing.
“Tell Me a Story”
We have conducted a brief survey of some of the main ways anecdotal
evidence for the existence of psychic powers, ghosts, alien abduction, monsters
and so on can be generated. It indicates why such evidence is almost entirely
worthless. You will remember in “But It Fits!” that we said evidence supports a
hypothesis to the extent that the
evidence is to be expected if the hypothesis is true, but not particularly
expected otherwise. The evidence has to be, in a certain sense,
“surprising.”
The problem with anecdotal evidence
for the such extraordinary claims is that, knowing what we do about how such
testimony tends to be generated, a great deal of it is to be expected anyway, whether the claims happen to be
true or not. The existence of quantities of such anecdotal evidence is not,
then, good evidence for the truth of such extraordinary claims.
Anecdotal evidence may be largely
worthless as evidence, but it can be highly persuasive. Humans love a story,
especially if it’s shocking, weird, or emotionally arresting. We enjoy
comedies, tragedies, stories of wrongs righted, of revenge, of ghosts, aliens.
One reason we find such stories appealing is that they tap into our tendency to
feel empathy with others. We enjoy imaginatively putting ourselves in the
subject’s position, imagining how it must have felt to exact that bloody
revenge, see a ghost, or be abducted by aliens. The more emotional impact the
story has, the more memorable it is.
As a consequence, a juicy story can
psychologically trump a dry statistic, even when the statistic is rather more
informative. The result of a double-blind clinical study of the efficacy of
prayer is a dull set of figures easily forgotten, whereas a handful of
emotionally arresting anecdotes about prayers answered may resonate with us for
a long time.
The Amazingly Persuasive
Power of Accumulated Anecdote
Pulling several anecdotes together can be particularly persuasive. A
shower of anecdotes often explains why people become convinced that certain
medical treatments are effective when they are not.
Bloodletting, popular from antiquity
until the late nineteenth century, was used to treat almost every disease. In
1799, George Washington asked to be bled after he developed a throat infection.
He died after large quantities of blood were removed. Benjamin Rush, a
physician and one of signatories to the Declaration of Independence, was, like
most of his contemporaries, entirely convinced that removing significant
quantities of blood from patients helped cure many ailments. Rush, like other
physicians, believed in the efficacy of bloodletting entirely on the basis of
anecdotes about people being bled and then recovering. Here he cites two
examples:
I bled a young man James Cameron, in
the autumn of 1794, four times between the 20th and 30th days of a chronic
fever, in consequence of a pain in the side, accompanied by a tense pulse,
which suddenly came on after the 20th day of his disease. His blood was sizy.
His pain and tense pulse were subdued by the bleeding and he recovered. I bled
the late Dr. Prowl twelve times, in a fever which continued thirty days, in the
autumn of the year 1800. I wish these cases to be attended by young
practitioners.9
It was not until the Parisian doctor
Pierre Louis conducted a controlled experiment in 1836—treating one group of
patients with pneumonia with aggressive bloodletting, and another group with
modest bloodletting—that the truth began to be revealed. The number of patients
who died after aggressive blood letting turned out to be greater.
Or consider homeopathy. In their
book Trick or Treatment, Simon Singh
and Edzard Ernst (the latter is both a professor of medicine and trained
homeopathic practitioner) conclude their assessment of the scientific evidence
regarding homeopathy, that “it would be fair to say that there is a mountain of
evidence to suggest that homeopathic remedies do not work.”10 So why do people think they work? Because of numerous anecdotes about the efficacy
of homeopathy. These anecdotes are, in reality, a result of people just getting
better anyway, the placebo effect, and other factors such as conventional
medicines also having an effect, subjects not wanting to disappoint those
interviewing them, and so on.
Many people also believe in the
power of intercessionary prayer to help people through medical crises. In 2006,
the American Heart Journal published the
results of a $2.4 million experiment involving 1,802 heart-bypass patients,
conducted under the leadership of Herbert Benson, a cardiologist who had
previously suggested that “the evidence for the efficacy of intercessionary
prayer is mounting” (so he was hardly biased against the claim that prayer
works). The results were clear cut: prayer had no beneficial effect on the
patients.11 Another large-scale trial of
patients undergoing angioplasty or cardiac catheterization also found prayer
had no effect.12 Unsurprisingly, such
studies don’t convince all those who believe in the power of prayer. After
these studies, Bob Barth, spiritual director of a Missouri prayer ministry
involved in the Benson prayer experiment, said, “A person of faith would say
that this study is interesting, but we’ve been praying a long time and we’ve
seen prayer work, we know it works, and the research on prayer and spirituality
is just getting started.”13
How did Bob Barth “know” prayers
works? Apparently in the same way Benjamin Rush “knew” that bloodletting
worked. On the basis of anecdotal evidence—that’s to say, various cases in
which the treatment was “seen to work.”
Christian Science
It’s often said that “the plural of anecdote is not data.” And yet a
pile of anecdotes can be made to look very much like solid “scientific” data.
Attempts have even been made to build a science on the basis of anecdotal
evidence. I will finish this chapter with a brief look at just such an
attempt—Christian Science.
As Caroline Fraser, a former
Christian Scientist herself, explains in her book God’s Perfect Child (on which much of this section is based)
Christian Scientists believe that they have rediscovered Christianity by
rediscovering the “exact method and means by which Jesus
healed. Only Christian Scientists believe they can duplicate those healings
systematically and repeatedly over a lifetime.”14
According to Mary Baker Eddy, the
founder of the Christian Science movement, matter does not exist and disease is
a product of the mind. A Christian Science practitioner’s training typically
involves two weeks of religious instruction. Practitioners are not trained to
diagnose illness, and in fact do not even believe in the reality of illness.
The treatment carried out by trained “practitioners” of Christian Science is
primarily prayer.
How do Christian Scientists know
they have discovered methods that work? Because of the “scientific” evidence
they have amassed over the years—tens of thousands of published testimonies of
cases in which the methods of Christian Science have been applied and people
have subsequently recovered. The “science” in Christian Science is meant
literally. The suggestion is that the thousands of testimonies or anecdotes
that the movement has accumulated over the years constitutes solid, statistical
evidence that Christian Science works.
By 1989, around fifty-three thousand
testimonies had been published in Christian Science periodicals. Early on, the
cured were allowed to write up their own cases. More recently, Christian
Science publications have prefaced such testimonials by saying:
The statements made in testimonies
and articles with regard to healing have been verified in writing by those who
can vouch for the integrity of the testifier or know of the healing. Three such
written verifications or vouchers are require before testimony can be
published.15
While this might strike some as being very “scientific,” the appearance
of scientific rigor is misleading.
Notice, first of all, that the
Christian Science movement ignores all
cases of failure. It counts only its “hits” and ignores all its “misses.”
Extraordinarily, no records of those who died after having received treatment
are kept. In their “Empirical Analysis” conducted by Christian Scientists of
over seven thousand treatments, the authors admitted that the study “does not
provide comparative cure or mortality rates, nor does it consider cases in
which healing prayer has not been effective.”16
This fact, all by itself, renders the evidence more or less worthless.
Worse still, many of the
testimonials look very dubious indeed. In 1954, the academic R. W. England of
the University of Pennsylvania published his analysis of a sample of five
hundred letters published in the Christian
Science Journal testifying to the power of Christian Science to heal. As
Fraser notes, England found that the self-diagnoses of Christian Scientists
were often unreliable:
The number of cancers, tumors,
broken bones, and cases of pneumonia and acute appendicitis which were
self-diagnosed by the writers seemed large. . . . It seems likely that most of
the more dramatic cures are due simply to mistaken diagnoses. In scores of
letters the writers describe how they broke their skulls, dislocated organs,
awoke in the night with pneumonia, decided mysterious lumps were cancers, or
found themselves in other ways serious victims of mortal mind. Their next move
was to begin divine treatment, with or without a practitioner’s aid. Elated and
gratified when the skulls mended, their organs returned to place, their
pneumonia and cancers vanished, they wrote letters of testimony to the Journal.17
Moreover, most letters concerned
fairly trivial—sometimes psychosomatic—conditions that tend to get better
anyway:
Most conspicuous was an apparent
ignorance of or indifference to the natural healing powers of the human body.
Thus, a vast number of minor ailments, ranging from athlete’s foot to the
common cold, were treated and cured by the application of Divine Truth.
Furthermore, there is, among the 500 communicants, considerable attention given
to types of disorders so insignificant as to be of practically no consequence
so far as one’s daily life is concerned. Chapped hands, lone warts, a burned
fingernail, hangnails, vague fleeting pains, a momentary dizziness were not
infrequently the “healings” for which testimony was given.18
It should be fairly obvious by now
why the “scientific evidence” for the efficacy of Christian Science is no such
thing. It’s just a vast collection of anecdotes—tens of thousands of
them—anecdotes of a sort that we might well expect to be produced by the kind
of mechanisms described in this chapter, whether or not Christian Science
actually works.
If Christian Science really worked,
then that fact could be established by a controlled experiment, as the
scientist Richard Feynman points out in his book, The Meaning of It All:
There is, in fact, an entire
religion that’s respectable, so called, that’s called Christian Science, that’s
based on the idea of faith healing. If it were true, it could be established,
not by the anecdotes of a few people but by careful checks.19
The Christian Science movement has
no interest in conducting such careful checks. They just stick with their
accumulation of anecdotes, which they dress
up as “science.”
Christian Science has cost lives.
People can and have died as a result of their rejecting conventional medical
services and plumping for the power of Christian Science instead. Hundreds,
perhaps thousands, of children have received Christian Science treatment rather
than conventional medicine. One of the most notorious cases is the 1979 incident
involving twelve-year old Michael Shram. As Michael started showing
increasingly serious gastric symptoms, his mother, a devout Christian
Scientist, decided to rely on the services, not of a doctor, but a Christian
Science practitioner. As a result, Michael died unnecessarily from a ruptured
appendix. According to one account, four days after developing symptoms,
Michael was vomiting violently and repeatedly. That night, he got up, washed
his face, and brushed his teeth. He then returned to bed saying, “It’s all
better Mommy,” and died. We don’t know how many children have died in this way,
because Christian Science keeps no record of its failures.
Christian Science is undoubtedly an
Intellectual Black Hole, and a potentially dangerous one at that. While other
of the eight mechanisms described in this book also play a role in giving
Christian Science a veneer of reasonableness and even scientific credibility,
it is Piling Up the Anecdotes that does the bulk of the work.
Notes
1. Quoted in
“Hidden Messages and the Bible Code” by David E. Thomas, which is published in Committee
for the Scientific Investigation of Claims of the Paranormal, Bizarre Cases (Amherst, NY: 2000), p. 124.
2. Ibid., p.
127.
3. Philip J. Klass, UFOs: The Public Deceived (Amherst NY, Prometheus Books 1983), p
83.
4. H. K. Beecher, “The Powerful Placebo,” J
Am Med Assoc 159, no. 17 (1955): 1602–6.
5. Jean
Piaget, Plays, Dreams and Imitation in
Childhood (New York: Norton, 1962), pp. 187–88.
6. See S. J. Lynn, T. Lock, E. F. Loftus, E. Krackow, and S. O.
Lilienfeld, “The Remembrance of Things Past: Problematic Memory Recovery
Techniques in Psychotherapy.” In S. O. Lilienfeld, S. J. Lynn, and J. M. Lohr,
eds., Science and Pseudoscience in Clinical Psychology (New York: Guilford,
2003). Also see: E. F. Loftus, “The Reality
of Repressed Memories,” American
Psychologist 48 (1993): 518–37; E. F. Loftus and K. Ketcham, The Myth of
Repressed Memory: False Memories and Accusations of Sexual Abuse (New York: St.
Martin’s Press, 1994).
7. Margaretta Fox Kane, quoted in R. B. Davenport, The Deathblow to Spiritualism (New York: Richardson, 2009), p. 76.
8. Harry Houdini,
A Magician Among the Spirits (New
York: Arno Press, 1972), p. 8.
9. From
“Defence of Bloodletting,” http://www.archive.org/stream/medicalinquiries04rush/medicalinquiries04rush_djvu.txt.
Accessed September 28, 2010.
10. Simon
Singh and Edzard Ernst, Trick or
Treatment (London: Bantam, 2009), p. 172.
11. H. Benson
et al., “Study of the Therapeutic Effects of Intercessionary Prayer (STEP) in
Cardiac Bypass Patients: A Multicenter Randomized Trial of Uncertainty and
Certainty of Receiving Intercessionary Prayer,” American Heart Journal 151 (2006): 934–42.
12. M. W. Krucoff
et al., “Music, Imagery, Touch, and Prayer as Adjuncts to Interventional Cardiac
Care: The Monitoring and Actualization of Noetic Trainings (MANTRA) II
Randomized Study,” Lancet 366 (2005):
211–17.
13. Quoted
in Richard Dawkins, The God Delusion
(London: Black Swan, 2006), p. 90.
14. Caroline
Fraser, God’s Perfect Child (New
York: Metropolitan Books, 1999), p. 417.
15. Quoted
in ibid., p. 421.
16. Quoted
in ibid., p. 425.
17. R. W.
England, “Some Aspects of Christian Science as Reflected in Letters of
Testimony,” American Journal of Sociology
59 (1954): 542. Quoted in Fraser, God’s
Perfect Child, pp. 432–33.
18. England,
“Some Aspects of Christian Science,” p. 451, quoted in Fraser, God’s Perfect Child, p. 432
19. Richard
Feynman, The Meaning of It All
(London: Penguin, 2007), p. 93.
Comments
It's easy to debunk dowsing as it's easy to interfere with the human energy system.
Homeopathy should be the main system of medicine http://whale.to/vaccine/homeopathy.html
"Homeopathy is wholly capable of satisfying the therapeutic demands of this age better than any other system or school of medicine."-----Dr. Charles Menninger M.D., Founder Menninger Clinic
I don't agree but it sure beats the medicine of Randi, Dawkins and co, that kills 2 million every year in the USA alone http://whale.to/c/big_brother.html
If the homeopaths cured no one they would still be 2 million bodies ahead before they even started!!
As to coincidences, there are no coincidences in medical land http://whale.to/a/just_a_coincidence_lie.html
and in reality http://whale.to/b/coincidence_h.html